Not much research has been conducted into the frequency of risky behaviour as children and adolescents become older. It is therefore not known whether risky behaviour increases after childhood and whether it decreases during developing adulthood. It is known that health-related risky behaviour (such as alcolhol consumption and smoking) highly increases during early adolescence (Van Dorsselaer et al., 2007; Monshouwer et al., 2004). With respect to risky road usage behaviour, more information is available for this age category about the differences between males and females. In questionnaires young men state more often than young women that they exhibit risky behaviour in traffic (Feenstra, Ruiter & Kok, 2010, Twisk et al., forthcoming). Peers also have an effect on risky behaviour. As it becomes increasingly important for them to belong to a group, adolescents tend to behave more hazardlously in the company of peers than when they are on their own (Crone, 2008; Nelis & Van Sark, 2010). Hardly any research has been conducted of the role of peer pressure in road user behaviour in the Netherlands. Research in the US among 16-17-year old car drivers shows that they behaved more risky when peers were passengers than when they were on their own. Peer pressure is not the only explanation for this behaviour. An alternative explanation is the increase of distraction due to the presence of passengers. See for the description of these studies archived SWOV Fact sheet Young drivers and their young passengers about this subject.
Although we actually have little insight into the exact nature and amount of deliberate risky road user behaviour of young adolescents, we do have knowledge about some of its contributing factors. These factors are: brain development, perceived invulnerability, social environment, impulsiveness, behavioural intentions and behaviour routines. These factors do not each determine an independent phenomenon, but address the risky behaviour from various perspectives.
Brain development
On account of recent insights into structural changes in the brain of young adolescents (Giedd, 2008), neuro-psychological research has focussed on the issue whether the rise in risky behaviour might be a result of this. Indeed, research showed that the areas of the brain that are responsible for impulse control, planning and integration of information (the control system) are still very much being developed during early adolescence. At the same time, the parts of the brain that are sensitive to stimuli, rewards, new experiences and the satisfaction of needs (the limbic system) are stimulated by the puberty hormones. The early increase of risky behaviour might be the result of this a-synchronic development, whereas its later decrease is the result of the increasing adult structure of the control system. Because of this, the activation from the limbic system is better regulated (Steinberg, 2008). At approximately 24 years of age, the brain reaches its ultimate adult structure. Also the difference in risky behaviour between boys and girls is occasionally traced back to this a-synchronic development. Due to the effect of the male sex hormone (testosteron) on the limbic system, the beginning of puberty is more intense, whereas the development of the control system is quite slower with boys than with girls (see Vlakveld, 2011 for an overview of the literature about this).
Aside from this negative effect on the safety of young adolescents, the structural changes and the greater plasticity of the brain have in fact a positive effect on the learning skills of young adolescents. This way, young adolescents, far more so than adults, can learn new skills, feel more challenged and can therefore reach levels of excellence more easily (Keating, 2007). This might also apply to learning the necessary skills for safe road user behaviour.
Perceived invulnerability
Other explanations for risky behaviour focus less on the physiological and more on the psychological factors, such as lack of knowledge, 'dangerous' notions, and (too) much confidence in one's own skills. An important factor in this context is estimating the risk that a hazard may occur, and the risk that it may happen to you. It is often assumed that young adolescents underestimate both and that, as a result, they behave more risky than adults. This would thus imply that interventions should particularly be directed towards correcting these 'wrong' notions. However, when adolescents are asked about the risk of dying because of risky behaviour, their estimates turn out to be unrealistically high. Considered against this background, providing the 'correct figures' or providing 'hard realistic' information would only be counter-productive (Reyna & Farley, 2006). See also SWOV Fact sheet Fear appeals and confronting information campaigns.
Social environment
During adolescence the social environment becomes more and more important. Whereas during childhood parental behaviour modelling is most effective, during adolescence the experiences with friends seem to guide behaviour in particular. This impression actually needs some correction, because although parents often have the impression that 'they are no longer listened to', research shows that they have more influence during adolescence than they are aware of (Bokhorst, Sumter & Westenberg, 2010; Nelis & Van Sark, 2010)
It is this interaction between adolescents and their environment that protects or, on the contrary, exposes them to hazards, not only in road usage, but also in other areas, such as health (unsafe sex or smoking and alcohol consumption; Jessor, 1992). In the Netherlands, indications have been found for multi-problem behaviour among adolescents. Junger, for example, found a connection between road crashes and criminality (Junger, Mesman & Meeus, 2003; Junger, Terlouw & Van der Heijden, 1994; Nieuwenhuijzen et al., 2009) and research by Wurst (2002) showed that a small part of the Dutch adolescents, often young men, behave risky in many areas. This implies that studies and interventions for the purpose of explaining and preventing risky behaviour should not only focus on isolated problem behaviour, but especially focus on the interaction between personal development and social environment.
Behavioural intention, impulsiveness and routine
People consider themselves as rational beings showing 'systematic' behaviour: we decide on something and we then carry it out. For this reason, in many behavioural theories behavioural intention is considered as the most important 'predictor' of behaviour. Indeed, many studies show that changes in behavioural intention lead to changes in actual behaviour, but also that this correlation is less strong than would be expected on the basis of the theory. We often have an intention, and sometimes we also carry it out. This happens, among other things, because 1) a great many of this kind of decisions develop less consciously than was earlier assumed, 2) a large part of the actions are more or less automatic (habits), and 3) the control over this behaviour is limited (Dijksterhuis, 2008; Webb & Sheeran, 2006). In fact, our behaviour is driven by two different systems: an intuitive system in which decisions are made quickly and automatically and a control system in which decisions develop systematically, consciously, but slowly (Kahneman, 2003).
The correlation between behavioural intention and actual behaviour is probably even weaker with adolescents than it is with adults. Firstly, this is due to the greater impulsiveness of adolescents (acting, not thinking) and, secondly, because their behaviour, to begin with, is less automatic and intuitive due to lack of experience and routine. Adolescents still consider things consciously, whereas adults practically immediately know without thinking what is the correct decision for them, based on earlier experiences. For instance, it shows from research in which adolescents and adults had to judge as quickly as possible whether certain behaviour was wise - for example, 'swimming with sharks' or 'setting your hair on fire' - that adolescents, more so than adults, have a longer reaction time and that their brain is more active before they judge this behaviour as unwise (Reyna & Farley, 2006). Moreover, with adolescents the outcome of the decision is more often situation-related than it is with adults. Examples are: adolescents that, once in the pub, cannot resist drinks offered, even though at home they had the intention not to drink alcohol, and adolescents that in a state of excitement more often make more risky decisions.
Although it is difficult to predict behaviour from behavioural intentions, attitude and knowledge, they do offer insight into differences between male and female. Thus, research shows that boys, more often than girls, find it less important, for instance, to stick to the rules, do not mind it so much to drink and drive and feel less responsible for others' safety (Feenstra, Ruiter & Kok, 2010; Twisk et al., forthcoming).